Various Audio: The Stanley Quencher madness officially jumped the shark last week when a California woman allegedly swiped 65 of these internet famous water bottles. Police …
Katy Milkman: Stanley Quencher tumblers: They're tall, stainless-steel, vacuum-insulated drinking containers with a handle and straw. They're also a cultural phenomenon. These $45 water bottles took the internet by storm around 2021.
Various Audio: … before the store even opened, to get her hands on the new pink Stanley tumbler …
Katy Milkman: People waited in line for hours outside Starbucks and Target and sprinted towards shelves holding the colorful containers. Cup swap groups cropped up and even resale markets. There were spreadsheets tracking launch dates for new products. There were even heists involving the theft of these popular mugs.
Various Audio: … allegedly stealing nearly $2,500 worth of these stainless steel status symbols …
Katy Milkman: On TikTok, it was hashtag #WaterTok. The cups appeared in the hands of women everywhere, which was surprising for a 100-year-old brand known mostly for camping thermoses. What propelled these tumblers into the hydration status symbols they became? Three marketers with an e-commerce site and an eye for what women want. The trio loved the Stanley Quencher, and so did the traffic on their site, the Buy Guide. When they learned their beloved bottle was about to be phased out, they stepped in and reached out to the company.
They convinced executives that these bottles were desirable and saleable. They demonstrated this with an Instagram post, where influencer Emily Maynard of The Bachelor fame shared her gifted Quencher. The company eventually came around to the idea of influencer marketing and let social media and its wave of seemingly organic endorsements do the rest. Soon, it looked like everyone loved the Stanley Quencher. What we saw were lifestyle creators and wellness influencers showing off their cups.
What we didn't necessarily see was the affiliate connection, and by the time people in our circle started carrying their own Stanleys, we'd lost sight entirely of what set these sales in motion. We figured they were truly a superior drinking vessel and not the product of a single social marketing campaign. The Quencher boosted Stanley's annual sales from $73 million in 2019 to $750 million in 2023.
Various Audio: I got a Stanley.
Katy Milkman: In this episode, we'll look at how one source of information, then echoed by seemingly independent sources, can distort our judgment.
Various Audio: Love my Stanley. Everyone should have one.
Katy Milkman: I'm Dr. Katy Milkman, and this is Choiceology, an original podcast from Charles Schwab. It's a show about the psychology and economics behind our decisions. We bring you true and surprising stories about high-stakes choices, and then we examine how these stories connect to the latest research in behavioral science. We do it all to help you make better judgments and avoid costly mistakes.
Joe Schwarcz: The cold is something that has bothered people ever since we've been around. It's a very unpleasant thing. For seven days, you are plagued with these symptoms of coughing, and sneezing, and whatever, and people have tried everything.
Katy Milkman: This is Joe.
Joe Schwarcz: Hi, I'm Joe Schwarcz.
Katy Milkman: Joe is a professor of chemistry at McGill University in Montreal, Canada. He's also the director of the University's Office for Science and Society. That office has a mandate to separate sense from nonsense, myth from fact.
Joe Schwarcz: Maimonides in the 12th century recommended chicken soup for the common cold, which of course, we still try today, even though there's no real documentation. People are very keen to jump on anything that offers a simple solution to a complex problem.
Katy Milkman: You may remember us covering this tendency to prefer a simple explanation in a previous episode of Choiceology. It's called the fallacy of the single cause, but what we're talking about today is actually quite different. It's about how we can get mixed up about credibility when there's a single source for a piece of information. This story starts with a famous scientist.
Joe Schwarcz: Here, you have a top-notch scientist telling you that he's got the answer. It's not surprising that you jump on that bandwagon.
Katy Milkman: The top-notch scientist is two-time Nobel Prize winner, American chemist and peace activist, Linus Pauling. The Bandwagon? Vitamin C.
Joe Schwarcz: Pauling was a brilliant scientist, starting from early childhood, where he built himself a lab in his house.
Katy Milkman: His house was in Portland, Oregon, where Pauling was born in 1901. He attended Oregon State University and then the California Institute of Technology, or Caltech. He earned his PhD in chemistry there, and then they hired him to join the faculty. By 1930, he had risen through the ranks to become a full professor.
Joe Schwarcz: He was very dedicated. At a very young age, he published very impactive studies, so this was not a quack by any means. He was a very legitimate scientist who certainly contributed a great deal to quantum mechanics and the chemical bond, and he almost got the structure of DNA before Crick and Watson. He thought that the DNA was three strands instead of two, but he was on track there all by himself. There's no question that Linus Pauling was a stellar scientist.
Katy Milkman: Joe Schwarcz was studying chemistry himself when he first learned about Pauling.
Joe Schwarcz: If you were into chemistry, you came across Linus Pauling and his classic textbook. He wrote what, at that time, was the definitive book on the chemical bond, and everything in chemistry is based on chemical bonds.
Katy Milkman: It was Pauling's work on chemical bonds that would win him a Nobel Prize in chemistry in 1954. Then later, he won a Nobel Peace Prize for his campaign against nuclear weapons and his advocacy for international peace.
Joe Schwarcz: He is the only person in the history of the Nobel Prize to win two unshared Nobel Prizes. That's something.
Katy Milkman: Pauling really was a remarkable scientist. He made fundamental contributions in a range of fields: chemistry, biology, and medicine. He's one of the most celebrated scientists of the 20th century, and one of the most trusted.
Joe Schwarcz: When you're a Nobel Prize winner, you're basically scientific royalty. Anything that you say gets listened to. When he started with his vitamin C business, of course it got immediately reported. It would've been ignored if it was anyone else, but this was Linus Pauling, who was arguably the greatest chemist of the times.
Katy Milkman: If you've ever heard of the healing properties of vitamin C, Linus Pauling is likely the reason why, and this is where it started.
Joe Schwarcz: Pauling was diagnosed with a type of kidney disease, and his physician recommended a diet, which was a low protein, low salt diet, on which he did much better. This initiated in his mind the concept of what came to be called ortho-molecular medicine. That was his brainchild. Ortho means "right," so ortho-molecular means using the right molecules. Because of his success with this treatment, which was nutritional, he thought that the body just needed all the right molecules in order to work efficiently.
Katy Milkman: In 1966 after an awards event where Pauling gave a lecture expressing a desire to live another 15 to 20 years, he ran into biochemist and vitamin C advocate Irwin Stone.
Joe Schwarcz: After his lecture, Stone came to him and told him about his ideas about how humans lost the evolutionary vestige of producing vitamin C. He was quick to buy into this. Even though there was no physical evidence, it just sounded right to him.
Katy Milkman: On Stone's advice, Pauling and his wife started taking 3000 milligrams of vitamin C daily. For context, you need about 10 to 20 milligrams a day to ward off scurvy, and the recommended daily allowance is closer to 100. At one point, Pauling and his wife were taking 18,000 milligrams daily. The mega dosing seemed to make them feel good. They claimed to catch fewer colds, and Pauling recommended everyone give this a try.
Joe Schwarcz: That is what gave rise to the original book, Vitamin C and the Common Cold, which of course took the world by storm. Because here was the most famous scientist in the world, arguably at that time, telling people that they should be taking vitamin C. Sales, of course, jumped. Vitamin producers noticed that right away. Sales kept increasing because so many magazine articles and newspaper articles talked about it. It's the kind of thing, you repeat something often enough, and it becomes true by repetition.
Katy Milkman: There's another Choiceology episode from a couple of seasons ago about how simple repetition can make something seem more true. This is called the illusory truth effect, but here, the information about vitamin C preventing or at least minimizing colds was repeated not just by Linus Pauling, but also by myriad other seemingly independent sources.
Joe Schwarcz: When you see articles all over the place, even though they all start from the same bit of information, but you just think, well, all of these articles, they can't all be wrong.
Katy Milkman: But they were wrong, because they all came from the same source, a reputable one at that, but as you'll see, an incorrect one.
Joe Schwarcz: Here was a guy who had published hundreds of papers in the scientific literature, obviously all peer-reviewed. He had a full understanding on how scientific methodology works, and here he was, publishing Vitamin C and the Common Cold based on anecdotes, his own and his wife.
Katy Milkman: The scientific community wasn't impressed by this lack of rigor, but they were intrigued by the claims.
Joe Schwarcz: Because of who Pauling was, they said, "OK, well, let's just take a look at it just in case there is something to this." There were a number of studies carried out, some on marine recruits, very good population to do such studies. If you have a Marine recruit and you tell them to jump, the only question they ask is how high. If they were told to take two grams of vitamin C a day, they did that. You had no doubt.
It didn't show that it prevented the common cold. There were a number of other studies done on that as well. Some showed a minimal effect, if you take regularly, a few hours less of the length of the cold. Not very encouraging, so the evidence just wasn't there.
Katy Milkman: In a perfect world, those inconclusive studies would've counteracted Pauling's claims, but the information was now snowballing, and businesses saw an opportunity.
Joe Schwarcz: By that time, the supplement industry had wound up, and when they start promoting things, it works, but there's just no evidence for Pauling's original contention, and certainly not of taking 18,000 milligrams. These concepts were scientifically valid. It's just that I think he went overboard.
Katy Milkman: Pauling didn't stop with the common cold. He went on to propose that the supplement had benefits for other diseases too. He wrote a book about cancer and vitamin C, and he linked vitamin C to heart health.
Joe Schwarcz: I actually heard him at a conference in Ottawa. There must have been a thousand people in the ballroom. He was the keynote speaker at a chemistry conference. He showed a graph. It showed an increased rate of heart disease and decreased intake of vitamin C in the population. One of the real bogeys in science, of course, is the correlation-causation business.
I remember sitting in there, thinking, here is this hero. He was a hero of mine, just throwing up that graph and saying conclusively that this demonstrated that taking more vitamin C would reduce heart disease. Nobody said anything. By that time, he was such an exalted figure that you didn't dare bring that up.
Katy Milkman: Every scientist there knew that correlation does not equal causation, and these wild claims have not stood up to rigorous testing. Why is this idea about vitamin C still out in the world and on our drugstore shelves? One answer is that people have heard it works so many times from so many seemingly different sources that they believe it must be true. For now, taking high doses of vitamin C is like taking a hope pill.
Joe Schwarcz: People live by hope. Anytime that there's some study which shows some potential, they will blow that out of proportion. It just keeps rolling along. There's so much money to be made.
Katy Milkman: Dr. Joe Schwarcz is the director of the Office for Science and Society at McGill University, where he is also a professor of chemistry. You can find more details about his work in the show notes and at schwab.com/podcast.
Have you ever had something recommended to you on social media? Say, a book? Someone says they've heard it's amazing, and then a few more people say this too, maybe in a group chat or another post. Maybe you thought, "Wow, everyone loves this book."
What if all those people heard it was amazing from the same source, a review in a popular magazine or maybe from Oprah's Book Club? To you, it sounded like a bunch of separate convergent opinions, but really just one common source has shared information. This can happen with books, restaurants, running shoes, stock tips, workplace rumors, and medical advice. When we don't realize that all of the information we're getting is connected, we can end up way off base when making decisions for ourselves.
Our failure to account for the fact that information from seemingly different sources might in fact be related or come from the same single source is called correlation neglect. Economists Florian Zimmermann and Benjamin Enke conducted experiments where participants tried to determine an unknown number based on guesses other people shared with them. In one condition, participants receive multiple independent guesses, five and seven, for example.
Then most participants wisely averaged across these guesses to come up with six. In another condition, though, participants were told one person's guess, five, for example, and the average of that guess with someone else's, let's say six, implying the other person's guess was seven. Here, participants failed to think clearly about what to do. Instead of guessing six under these circumstances, which would've been the right path, many guessed the equivalent of 5.5, averaging the five and six, and showing a bias where they treated correlated information as if it were independent.
Florian Zimmermann joins me today to talk about the implications of this research. Florian is a professor of economics at the University of Bonn in Germany. Hi, Florian. Thank you so much for joining me today.
Florian Zimmermann: Hi, Katy. My pleasure.
Katy Milkman: I'm really excited to talk with you about correlation neglect, and I was hoping we could just start with a definition.
Florian Zimmermann: Yes, of course. By correlation neglect, we mean that if people get the information from multiple sources, and these sources are somehow connected, thereby generating a correlation, if they neglect this correlation, if they ignore this correlation in the belief formation process, this is then what we mean by correlation neglect.
Katy Milkman: What are some situations where we might get information from multiple sources and they might be correlated, or uncorrelated, just so our listeners can get an intuition for what you have in mind?
Florian Zimmermann: Yes. Think about forming a belief, an opinion about something, and you are perhaps consulting your social network. You ask a bunch of friends. This would already be a situation where you are getting information from multiple sources. Now, this is also an example where I think oftentimes information will be correlated in an interesting way. For instance, picture a situation where there is a friend in your social network, and this friend is very influential in that network, meaning that he talks to many other people in your network.
By talking to these other people in your network, probably the opinion or the information you will get from these friends will partly reflect what they heard from this very influential friend. We often call this a common source of information, and this information or the knowledge that this very important friend in your social network has is getting partially repeated by other people in your social network. That creates a specific correlation structure.
Katy Milkman: Say I hear from three different friends about an amazing financial opportunity, and I think, "Wow, this must be really good," but a correlation structure would be something like they all heard it from the same central friend, and then they repeated that information to me. To me, it felt like it was three independent people telling me, "Wow, this is a great investment opportunity," but in reality, it was actually just one who had that kind of diffusion. Is that what you have in mind?
Florian Zimmermann: That's exactly right. It wouldn't even need to be that they exactly repeat what the friend said. It would be enough if they're sort of influenced by what this common source of information, this common friend says. That's exactly right. Yeah.
Katy Milkman: Got it. Am I right in thinking that it doesn't have to just be through social networks, that this could happen through other channels as well? If I listen to the radio and watch the news at night, is that another situation where I might need to worry about this?
Florian Zimmermann: Absolutely, yes. Think of a situation where there's a very influential expert, and different journalists are sort of partially repeating what this very influential expert is saying. Then by consuming different news outlets, you're going to be exposed to correlated information.
Katy Milkman: If I neglect that, it would mean I treat those as if they're both news sources, both the radio program and the news program, giving me some warning or suggesting some course of action. If I am neglecting that they both got the same expert to give them that guidance, then I might put too much emphasis on that information that gets higher quality than it is?
Florian Zimmermann: Yes.
Katy Milkman: OK.
Florian Zimmermann: If you ignore this correlation, you're going to put too much weight on this common source of information. If this common source of information is overly optimistic on some issue, you're going to be overly optimistic, and vice versa. If this person is going to be or this news source is going to be overly pessimistic, you're going to be overly pessimistic.
Katy Milkman: This is really interesting. It seems really relevant to so many different situations. I'm wondering if you can tell us a little bit about what you think causes correlation neglect?
Florian Zimmermann: Yes. We were, of course, extremely curious to delve into this. Perhaps it's useful to think about a sort of very, very simple framework of what could go wrong if you're dealing with correlated information. The first step, if as a respondent in our little experiment, you approach this task, it might be that you simply fail to notice the correlation structure. There's so many things happening, so many different numbers you're seeing, you just don't even notice that there is this correlation structure.
That could be sort of the first step where you're failing. The second step could be that, well, you notice it, but what you don't notice is that actually, by ignoring this correlation structure, you are making a mistake. You are systematically biasing your own beliefs. That could also be happening. Noticing might not be enough. You also need to realize that there's a problem. Then the third step would be, well, suppose you've realized that there's a problem. You also need to know how to solve it, so how to correct for the problem.
In principle, each of these three steps could go wrong. We've conducted a bunch of experiments that essentially tell us that it's not the third part. It's not that people don't know how to correct for this bias. It must be something more along the lines of either failing to notice the correlation structure, or failing to notice that there is a problem with this correlation structure.
Katy Milkman: I also seem to recall that you found not everyone exhibits this bias. Am I remembering that correctly, that you can sort of disentangle the types of people who are most susceptible to it?
Florian Zimmermann: That's right. If we sort of were to classify our participants, essentially, we would get two groups: one group that really fully neglects or ignores the correlation structure and another group that actually does a fairly good job at incorporating the correlation structure and correcting for it.
Katy Milkman: That's really interesting. Do your studies give you any hints about what we could do to eliminate or reduce this neglect in the people who don't behave properly? What might change that so that they could be more optimal in their decisions?
Florian Zimmermann: Yeah, so I think there's a couple of hints from additional experiments we've done, and they all point to the same thing, which is that if you really focus their attention on the correlation structure, and there's different ways to do it, but if you really put an alarm that says, "Look, be careful, watch out for this particular structure," then they actually do the right thing.
I think one implication would be that in sort of real-world situations where we might worry that people are ignoring correlations, our results would suggest that in these situations, really sort of hitting them on the head with, "Be mindful of this particular structure of your information environment," that that already might help in some situations at least.
Katy Milkman: Reminding people, "Hey, if you heard this information from Sarah, make sure you think about whether or not it ultimately came from Brian," or "If you hear this from The Boston Globe, you should check and see if it was the same sources as the NPR show you listened to this morning," something along those lines. Kind of a reminder.
Florian Zimmermann: That's exactly right. If you think about the underlying psychology, it seems that in such situations where people consume information from multiple sources, they are already sort of overwhelmed by the information they're getting, which makes them very susceptible for simply ignoring the specific aspect of the information structure.
Then as you say, by reminding them, by really focusing them on the importance of this specific aspect, that might already help at least some people.
Katy Milkman: That's great. I'm curious if you do anything differently in your life now that you know about this bias and have studied it thoroughly?
Florian Zimmermann: That's a great question. I'm sure I do. I guess kind of a boring answer is that I'm more often than before really simply ask myself, "Where does a certain news outlet or perhaps even a friend I'm talking to, where does he or she get his information from?" I'm questioning that a little bit more.
Katy Milkman: That's a great answer. I love that. I so appreciate you taking the time to speak with me today. This was really interesting. I learned a lot, and I know our listeners will be grateful as well, so thank you so much.
Florian Zimmermann: No, no. Thank you, Katy. It was a pleasure.
Katy Milkman: Florian Zimmermann is a professor of economics at the University of Bonn, and along with Harvard economist, Ben Enke, he uncovered the problem of correlation neglect. You can find links to his work in the show notes and at schwab.com/podcast.
There's no shortage of hype—or, occasionally, hysteria—in the financial media when popular narratives get recycled. For objective, data-driven analysis of both the stock and bond markets, check out the Schwab original podcast On Investing. Hosted by Chief Investment Strategist Liz Ann Sonders and Chief Fixed Income Strategist Kathy Jones, new episodes of On Investing drop every Friday. You can find them at schwab.com/OnInvesting or in your favorite podcasting app.
Since learning about correlation neglect, I found myself constantly asking, "Where is this information really coming from?" When two colleagues raise the same concern, I pause to consider, "Are these independent observations, or did they both witness the same incident?" When I see the same product hyped across multiple social media accounts, I ask, "Are these truly separate endorsements, or are they all drawing from the same original post?"
The danger of correlation neglect is that it can make weak evidence feel overwhelming. We assume consensus where there's really just repetition, but simply asking, "What's the source, and is it the same?" when we hear related news, gossip, or advice can help us avoid placing too much faith in, say, vitamin C. It's a small mental habit that can make a big difference in how we judge what's credible and what's not. Of course, I would be remiss if I didn't point out the risk that your knowledge of this bias could turn into a bias itself.
It would be a mistake to become overly skeptical of high-quality, repeatedly and independently validated information, say, on vaccine safety, or the risk of buying penny stocks, or the dangers of climate change. The trick is to stay curious. Don't blindly trust repetition, but don't dismiss consensus, either. Instead, for important decisions, try to trace the source of what you're hearing. Understanding where information comes from is one of the surest ways to make better decisions.
You've been listening to Choiceology, an original podcast from Charles Schwab. If you've enjoyed the show, we'd be really grateful if you'd leave us a review on Apple Podcasts, a comment or rating on Spotify or YouTube, or feedback wherever you listen. You can also follow us for free in your favorite podcasting app. If you want more of the kinds of insights we bring you on Choiceology about how to improve your decisions, you can order my book, How to Change, or sign up for my monthly newsletter, Milkman Delivers, on Substack.
Next time, I'll speak with Cornell University psychologist Tom Gilovich. He'll explain our tendency to vastly overestimate the likelihood that other people will notice or pay attention to minor flaws in our appearance, decisions, or behavior. I'm Dr. Katy Milkman. Talk to you soon.